Sunday, April 17, 2011

The Research on Remediation

"Developmental education is minimally effective, at best."
Community College Research Center National Center for Postsecondary Research Presentation


I read a posting by "Dean Dad" on the Inside Higher Education website the day it was posted and have been pondering it since. It is appended below.

While at the Higher Learning Commission Conference in Chicago this weekend, I had a moment to discuss it over a drink with Amy Penne and Kris Young. Apparently, it has been understood by some for quite a while now that assessment testing and mandatory placement, primarily in mathematics, reading, and writing has nothing to do with success (pass rates) in developmental courses.

"Students who simply disregarded the placement and went directly into college-level courses did just as well as students who did as they were told."

I don't think I can adequately communicate how much this disturbs me. Does this mean that students who score in the bottom 5% of the placement tests do as well as those who score in the top 5%, if they end up in the same class? 

At Parkland College, we painstakingly and methodically assess each new student, provide a large collection of resources (Maybe some RSA Animation or Khan videos?) to assist in preparation for the assessment, counsel and advise students into a variety of programs and services congruent with national best practices. 

Yet, according to the research, nothing improves pass rates more effectively than shortening (and intensifying) the instructional experience. 


A few of the responses to the findings about assessment/placement testing:

1. The effect is based on the fact that those students in higher level classes are taught based on the assumption that they have forgotten everything they "learned" in the prerequisite course.

2. Many students take placement tests without understanding their purpose or high-stakes nature – they experience assessment as a “one-shot deal.”


3. "Letting students go directly into a course for which they are not prepared might improve retention and graduation rates, but that approach is likely to cause another problem. When a compassionate instructor has a lot of these students in class, he or she is likely to slow down and help them, both in and out of class. Then the class may not be able to get to all of the material in the depth it is supposed to cover. This creates an additional problem in upper division courses because the students are not ready at that point either. The problem cascades upward.

Over time, instructors are worn down and the course is watered down. The instructor spends far more time outside of class helping these students, diverting them from their other duties such as class prep and grading or service and research (the latter being a significant problem in universities where faculty have obligations other than teaching)."  - A comment from a
reader on Insider Higher Ed. 

4. "This finding is simply a variation on the common sense idea that having contemporaneous support outside the classroom will improve learning outcomes. Guess what's another way that can happen? By paying the people who teach these courses a living wage with benefits so that they can spend an appropriate amount of time meeting with their students outside the classroom (in private offices, not at McDonald's), giving the students the academic support and attention that each student needs." -  Another IHE reader 

5. "I see students everyday who cannot understand the test instructions, let alone "pass" the test. I don't see how a professor could see to their needs while also meeting the expectations of the better prepared students." - Same as above

An interesting approach:




Dean Dad's posting:

Remedial Levels


A few weeks ago I promised a piece on remedial levels. It’s a huge topic, and my own expertise is badly limited. That said...

Community colleges catch a lot of flak for teaching so many sections of remedial (the preferred term now is “developmental”) math and English. (For present purposes, I’ll sidestep the politically loaded question of whether ESL should be considered developmental.) In a perfect world, every student who gets here would have been prepared well in high school, and would arrive ready to tackle college-level work.

This is not a perfect world. And given the realities of the K-12 system, especially in low-income areas, I will not hold my breath for that.

Many four-year colleges and universities simply exclude the issue by having selective admissions. Swarthmore doesn’t worry itself overly much about developmental math; if you need a lot of help, you just don’t get in. But community colleges are open-admissions by mission; we don’t have the option to outsource the problem. We’re where the problem gets outsourced.

I was surprised, when I entered the cc world, to discover that course levels and pass rates are positively correlated; the ‘higher’ the course content, the higher the pass rate. Basic arithmetic -- the lowest level developmental math we teach -- has a lower pass rate than calculus. The same holds in English, if to a lesser degree.

At the League for Innovation conference a few weeks ago, some folks from the Community College Research Center presented some pretty compelling research that suggested several things. First, it found zero predictive validity in the placement tests that sentence students to developmental classes. Students who simply disregarded the placement and went directly into college-level courses did just as well as students who did as they were told. We’ve found something similar on my own campus. Last year, in an attempt to see if our “cut scores” were right, I asked the IR office and a math professor to see if there was a natural cliff in the placement test scores that would suggest the right levels for placing students into the various levels of developmental math. I had assumed that higher scores on the test would correlate with higher pass rates, and that the gently-slanting line would turn vertical at some discrete point. We could put the cutoff at that point, and thereby maximize the effectiveness of our program.

It didn’t work. Not only was there no discrete dropoff; there was no correlation at all between test scores and course performance. None. Zero. The placement test offered precisely zero predictive power.

Second, the CCRC found that the single strongest predictor of student success that’s actually under the college’s control -- so I’m ignoring gender and income of student, since we take all comers -- is length of sequence. The shorter the sequence, the better they do. The worst thing you can do, from a student success perspective, is to address perceived student deficits by adding more layers of remediation. If anything, you need to prune levels. Each new level provides a new ‘exit point’ -- the goal should be to minimize the exit points.

I’m excited about these findings, since they explain a few things and suggest an actual path for action.

Proprietary U did almost no remediation, despite recruiting a student body broadly comparable to a typical community college. At the time, I recall regarding that policy decision pretty cynically, especially since I had to teach some of those first semester students. Yet despite bringing in students who were palpably unprepared, it managed a graduation rate far higher than the nearby community colleges.

I’m beginning to think they were onto something.

This week I saw a webinar by Complete College America that made many of the same points, but that suggested a “co-requisite” strategy for developmental. In other words, it suggested having students take developmental English alongside English 101, and using the developmental class to address issues in 101 as they arise. It would require reconceiving the developmental classes as something closer to self-paced troubleshooting, but that may not be a bad thing. At least that way students will perceive a need for the material as they encounter it. It’s much easier to get student buy-in when the problem to solve is immediate. In a sense, it’s a variation on the ‘immersion’ approach to learning a language. You don’t learn a language by studying it in small chunks for a few hours a week. You learn a language by swimming in it. If the students need to learn math, let them swim in it; when they have what they need, let them get out of the pool.

I’ve had too many conversations with students who’ve told me earnestly that they don’t want to spend money and time on courses that “don’t count.” If they go in with a bad attitude, uninspired performance shouldn’t be surprising. Yes, extraordinary teacherly charisma can help, but I can’t scale that. Curricular change can scale.

This may seem pretty inside-baseball, but from the perspective of someone who’s tired of beating his head against the wall trying to improve student success rates without lowering standards, these findings offer real hope. It may be that the issue isn’t that we’re doing developmental wrong; the issue is that we’re doing it at all.

There’s real risk in moving away from an established pattern of doing things. As Galbraith noted fifty years ago, if you fail with the conventional approach, nobody holds it against you; if you fail with something novel, you’re considered an idiot. The “add-yet-another-level” model of developmental ed is well-established, with a legible logic of its own. But the failures of the existing model are just inexcusable. Assuming three levels of remediation with fifty percent pass rates at each -- which is pretty close to what we have -- only about 13 percent of the students who start at the lowest level will ever even reach the 101 level. An 87 percent dropout rate suggests that the argument for trying something different is pretty strong.

Wise and worldly readers, have you had experience with compressing or eliminating developmental levels? If so, did it work?



This is the part of the show where you talk. See all that empty space below? That is the space where you tell me (and everyone else) what you think.

(12,750)

12 comments:

  1. So why did we start doing developmental classes anyway? It seems to me it was because just doing nothing wasn't working. And we're suppose to go back to that? I'm confused.

    Maybe our placement tests aren't that good. Would that account for some of the findings?

    ReplyDelete
  2. Excellent topic that can lead to honest discussion. If the research is accurate and developmental classes aren't working, why continue, or why continue in the way they are currently being offered?

    ReplyDelete
  3. After reading the comments from Dean's Dad posting I'm less confused. I thought the comments regarding alternative ways of handling developmental courses were interesting and the options worth pursuing.

    I'd also like to know what some of the Parkland faculty who teach our developmental courses think about this.

    As an outsider to developmental ed, the studies seem so counterintuitive I keep having a hard time believing them. Yet,my teaching experience and my gut keep whispering to me they may be right.

    ReplyDelete
  4. This is definitely worth researching further. I do like the idea of corequisite coursework, especially in English. (Harder to implement in math, but I'm sure we could find a way.)

    That said, we must also be careful about interpreting the results of this research. The developmental courses may not be the sole problem--there are lots of factors at play here in these students' lives. Helping with those other issues might increase the success of dev ed classes. (Which is what we are trying to do with our Center for Academic Success.) There is also the problem of aggregate data, which can hide success rates in some groups that are hidden by lack of success in other groups.

    Very important issues here. Always worth taking a fresh look at our approach. This may involve looking at course sequences, pedagogy, as well as support services.

    ReplyDelete
  5. Charles Shapland Tom, I think you are right in your thought that placing "all comers" in the same classes would serve to slow the classes down, and as a consequence reduce the level of content that the upper level students receive. I think the argument that can be made using the data is that what is needed are, using math as an example, say algebra 1 instead of dropping someone into remedial math, AND an additional course offering, say algebra 1 honors, that allows the better prepared students to get a deeper dive into the material, an not be held back by the less prepared
    students who consume the time and effort of the instructors. The assumption may be correct, and there may be better pass rates by moving students ahead in terms of course work, but the course of action that is suggested may not be simply placing all of the students in the same
    courses for the reasons that you suggest.

    ReplyDelete
  6. Interesting insight into the issues you face. After having spent eight years teaching mixed ability classes at the secondary level, I'm fairly confident that instructors will have to alter the pace once a large enough percentage of under-prepared students are enrolled in their classes. The missing piece in the research cited seems to be the characteristics of those students who don't do "as they were told." Perhaps they are more motivated to challenge themselves?
    Joseph Lyons

    ReplyDelete
  7. The "Merit" program here at Parkland (in biology) and at U of I (chemistry, biology) offers extra class hours (for 1 hour of credit) to work together in groups on course material. It is targeted at specific populations to help those who statistically fall behind their peers. I TAed for it during grad school and found the "just in time troubleshooting" a great approach. I don't currently teach developmental courses do I'm not sure if it would work for everyone.

    ReplyDelete
  8. Okay, those SOAR students are coming in ... but my observation is that when you do the acceleration thing you have better results because a whole huge chunk of students doesn't get counted at all. There are *****so***** many other variables involved -- all developmental ed is not the same, but this acts as if that is so.

    ReplyDelete
  9. Okay, it's quiet down here... here are some assorted observations.

    When I first came here, I spent some hours in the summer crunching numbers. There *was* a reasonably strong correlation between assessment scores and success in developmental courses. I don't remember whether I tagged 098 or 099, but in one year, students who scored above 70 on the Comprehension diagnostic of the Compass score had a pretty high success rate. (Vocabulary subtest had almost no correlation... students with high vocab and low comprehension had a genuine negative correlation.)
    This tells me that other folks' research findings may not apply here.
    Some other "variables" -- what other subtle "screening" happens? Math, writing, reading -- if you separate them, what does that do to your pass rates?
    Are the pass rates actually good in those "accelerated" classes -- oh, and what about graduation? If they all stink, but one's a little better, then perhaps it's really that accelerating means, simply, less B.S.
    I would quit my job if students could take whatever math course they wanted to. When I'm tutoring students, most of the time they have the conceptual background to have a chance of learning what's expected. (I can work around you not knowing times tables, but if you're trying to take 098 and you don't understand that 4 + -1 is 3, it's a struggle, and don't even bring up 0.) In my experience, students are excellent at either grossly over- or under- estimating their math competence.
    Frankly, though, conclusions made about "developmental education" as if it's all one thing doesn't make any sense.

    ReplyDelete
  10. (clarification -- if the acceleration is only a little better than the other, then it's less of something that's fundamentally useless; shouldn't we be trying to figure out what would be useful?)

    ReplyDelete
  11. Just some quick off-the-top initial comments:
    1. What represents a "typical" community college? Is Proprietary U located in an urban or rural or mixed setting?

    2. Developmental courses began as a way to bring students up to speed so that they would not feel marginalized as they progressed into college-level courses. At least, that's my rough understanding of the history of these types of courses.

    3. Greater understanding has come to light for many students who viewed the COMPASS test as just a minor irritant/hoop in order to be accepted for admission. However, more work can be done in this area. Still encounter those who believe the test doesn't matter that much or they get tired of the length and just push through it with little regard to the consequences--for example, ending up in developmental classes.

    4. Some students in Accelerated Composition seem to possess high vocab and also can put together sentences which, technically, seem okay. However, upon further examination, my own experience has been that some of these same students may have fared better in developmental classes.

    5. How might an Algebra I Honors course help those who are already relatively equipped to tackle algebra? Taking the all comers approach allows the instructor to devote time to those who really need her/his help, as opposed to those who may need nominal assistance. I also think we have to be careful about creating an atmosphere of intellectual elitism by offering such classes. Developmental students already assume (wrongly or rightfully so) that they are somehow stigmatized once they enter these courses.

    One summer I was given the task of teaching two combined levels of developmental English. When the expectations are clear and not diluted, students often rise and exceed expectations. Having said that, developmental ed classes are not "one size fits all" so Proprietary U may have been looking at factors somewhat alien to our own experiences.

    Finally, some of the external factors such as gender, income, race, class, as well as situations that factor into student attendance, persistence, willingness to seek resources, etc. have to be taken into account because they are, more often than not, prime factors in drop rates for these courses, not to mention many of our college-level courses as well.

    Some of my colleagues at research institutions such as University of Kentucky, University of Illinois, Southern Illinois University at Carbondale and Edwardsville are experiencing these same sorts of problems. Many universities create developmental courses under such titles as Basic Skills or something similarly synonymous, but they suffer attrition rates that closely match community colleges.

    ReplyDelete
  12. Anyone who continues to use the word remedial is clearly not up to date with the latest scholarship or reasearch in the developmental education field. A more reliable source is necessary: http://www.mdrc.org/publications/504/full.pdf

    -Rochelle
    -14 years experience in developmental education
    -member of the National Association for Developmental Educators
    -graduate of the National Center for Developmental Education, Kellogg program

    ReplyDelete

R,P,& C + Standards